Breading Mice Should You Take Out Old Babys When New Ones Arive
J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2009 Sep; 48(5): 492–498.
Published online 2009 Sep.
Convenance and Housing Laboratory Rats and Mice in the Same Room Does Non Affect the Growth or Reproduction of Either Species
Kathleen R Pritchett-Corning
1Inquiry Models and Services, Charles River, Wilmington, Massachusetts
2Department of Comparative Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
Fon T Chang
3AstraZeneca, Waltham, Massachusetts
Michael F W Festing
4Understanding Animal Inquiry, London, United Kingdom
Received 2009 Mar 17; Revised 2009 Apr sixteen; Accepted 2009 May 17.
Abstruse
Few information exist regarding the effects of long-term housing of rats and mice in the same secondary enclosure. Historical reproductive and growth data were compared for colonies of mice and rats maintained in open up-topped cages in either unmarried-species or dual-species barrier rooms. This analysis included reproductive parameters (litter size at birth, litter size at weaning, and pups missing at weaning) collected from 33 colonies of mice comprising 500 to 38,500 breeding females and 28 colonies of rats totaling 350 to four,600 breeding females, and representative samples from 28 colonies of each species were analyzed for weight gain from weaning to adulthood. The presence or absence of the other species was not associated with statistically pregnant differences in weight gain or any of the reproductive parameters. These results advise that breeding colonies of rats and mice of the same wellness status can be housed in the same room without a negative effect on the growth and reproduction of either species.
The Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals suggests that animals should be physically separated by species. 22 This practice is recommended to prevent transmission of disease betwixt species and to remove interspecies conflict as a source of anxiety or behavioral change. The Guide too suggests, however, that species that are "behaviorally compatible" and have similar health status may be housed in the same room. The Guide provides reasons and references for separating rabbits and guinea pigs, New World and Old World primates, and several species of New Earth primates, all based on the possibility of disease transmission. In these cases, 1 species is an asymptomatic carrier of a disease that may have serious consequences for the other. 22 All the same, the Guide does not specifically mention rats and mice, which are oft housed together in the same room in breeding and research facilities.
Recommendations against housing rats and mice together typically practise non refer to illness transmission as a primary consideration, 22 even though rats and mice are susceptible to some of the same bacterial and viral infections. However, rats and mice are generally of a similar health status in modern animal facilities, especially if purchased from a commercial breeder or reared in the same room in a research facility. Concern about housing these 2 species together generally is based on the ascertainment that rats may prey on mice, 23 with the supposition that mice would notice it stressful to live in the presence of rats, which could be predators. All the same, neither rats nor mice are master predators; more accurately they are opportunistic generalists. They eat what is available, which may or may not include food obtained past predation. The suggestion that rats are stressed past the presence of mice or other potential prey has not been advanced. Most published reports of housing mice and rats together are brusk-term exposure studies, in which mice are housed in the same room or cage as rats for fewer than 30 d. five,6,ix,x,25,29,44 Published work evaluating the effects of long-term housing of rats and mice together was absent from the literature. Here we address this gap.
For many years at Charles River facilities, mice and rats have been bred and housed together in the aforementioned room equally a routine production procedure. These colonies include breeders (up to approximately 9 mo of age) and stock animals (more often than not a maximum of 10 to 12 wk of historic period). Mice and rats are housed together primarily to facilitate the best utilise of available space while allowing stocks and strains with the aforementioned coat colors to be bred and raised in carve up rooms to minimize the risk of genetic contamination. All animals within a particular barrier room share an identical health condition, and then transmission of infectious agents betwixt species is not a business. All bulwark rooms discussed in this newspaper are maintained as closed colonies, in which animals remain in the room of their birth. A small number of breeders are introduced every few years as part of genetic management systems.
At Charles River, animate being husbandry staff regularly collects production information for evaluation of colony performance and colony size management. In improver, weight studies are conducted regularly of animals from weaning to 15 wk of historic period. These metrics allow comparing of growth and reproduction of mice bred and reared either with or without rats in the same room and of rats bred and reared with or without mice in the same room. If stress levels are high (for case, due to the presence of a predator–prey relationship), reproduction and growth of animals are probable to be affected. If the population to be evaluated is sufficiently large, subtle behavioral effects can be expected to announced equally differences in growth or reproduction, which would exist constitute when gross measures of these complex behavioral chains are evaluated.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
This study was conducted past using historical information collected from a database, therefore blessing from an institutional care and use committee was not sought for analysis of the data. All rats and mice discussed in this study were bred and housed in AAALAC-accredited facilities, and the overall protocol for breeding and production (Charles River, Wilmington, MA), which includes weight studies, was approved by the institutional care and use commission. Environmental conditions within rooms were maintained at 21° ± ane °C (seventy° ± 2 °F) with fifty% ± 20% relative humidity and ventilated at a minimum of 15 HEPA-filtered air changes per hour. Animals were kept on a 12:12 light:dark wheel and provided ad libitum access to water and feed (Lab Diet 5K52 or Lab Diet 5L79, Purina Mills, Richmond, IN). Breeding rooms analyzed in this written report averaged approximately 2400 ft2. Actual layouts of each room differed slightly, so a general description of a bulwark room is provided. Mice and rats tended to be grouped within rooms because mouse and rat cages differ in size, therefore animals were not housed on the aforementioned rack within a room. Mice were housed in solid-bottomed cages, with wood-shaving or chipped-wood bedding. Rats were housed on either wire-bottomed cages (stock or breeding cages) or solid-bottomed cages (maternity cages) containing a woods-shaving product. All animals were housed socially in groups of two to twenty, with the exception of female person rats in late pregnancy and male person rats or mice used in timed mating programs. Bedding for all cages was changed as needed, merely at least weekly, and all cages were open to the room surroundings (that is, no microisolation or ventilated caging).
Mice were from colonies that tested negative for the following viral agents: Sendai virus, pneumonia virus of mice, mouse hepatitis virus, infinitesimal virus of mice, mouse parvovirus, mouse norovirus, Theiler murine encephalomyelitis virus, reovirus type iii, mouse adenovirus, polyoma virus, One thousand virus, mouse cytomegalovirus, rotavirus, mouse thymic virus, lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus, hantavirus, lactate dehydrogenase elevating virus, and Ectromelia virus. All rats were from colonies that tested negative for the following viral agents: Sendai virus, pneumonia virus of mice, sialodacryoadenitis virus, Kilham rat virus, H1 virus, rat minute virus, reovirus, lymphocytic choriomeningitis virus, hantavirus, mouse adenovirus, rat respiratory virus, rat theilovirus, and rat parvovirus. In add-on, colonies were free of the following bacterial and fungal agents: Bordetella bronchiseptica, cilia-associated respiratory bacillus, Citrobacter rodentium, Clostridium piliforme, Corynebacterium kutscheri, Encephalitozoon cuniculi, Helicobacter spp., Mycoplasma pulmonis, Pasteurella pneumotropica, Salmonella spp., Streptobacillus moniliformis, and Streptococcus pneumoniae. In add-on, rats and mice were free of endoparasites and ectoparasites. The genetics of both inbred and outbred colonies are managed in reference to security colonies held at a cardinal location in isolators, and colonies participate in a genetic standardization program.
Source of raw data.
Data for the 2 parameters examined—reproduction and growth—were held in 2 different systems, thus explaining the differing data retrieval periods available. Reproductive data were nerveless from an inventory organisation that records colony functioning and manages inventory of animals involved in the production of animals for sale. Weight data were collected from a organization designed for the management of repeating weight studies performed regularly on convenance colonies. The data were not specifically recorded for this study.
Data were obtained on the breeding operation of 33 colonies of both inbred and outbred mice maintained either with or without rats in the same creature room (Table 1). There were no known systematic differences betwixt colonies of the aforementioned strain or stock autonomously from the fact that the mice were bred either in the presence or absence of breeding colonies of rats. Data also were obtained for 28 colonies of outbred rats bred in rooms with or without mice (Table 2). The smallest mouse colony examined contained a breeding population of 500 breeding females, and the largest contained 38,500 breeding females. Rat colonies ranged in size from 350 to 4600 breeding females. All colony numbers are approximate.
Tabular array one.
Litter size and mortality among mouse strains housed with or without rats
Strain or stock | Housed with or without other species? | Hateful no. of pups per litter at nativity | Mean no. of pups weaned per littera | Mean no. of pups missing per litterb |
BALB/cAnNCrl | with | 6.75 | 6.49 | 0.34 |
BALB/cAnNCrl | without | vi.fourscore | vi.51 | 0.31 |
C57BL/6JN | with | v.33 | v.43 | 0.51 |
C57BL/6JN | without | half-dozen.12 | 6.05 | 0.71 |
C57BL/6JN | without | 5.92 | 5.51 | 0.92 |
C57BL/6JN | without | six.lx | vi.71 | 0.25 |
C57BL/6NCrl | with | vii.41 | 7.34 | 0.17 |
C57BL/6NCrl | with | 5.58 | 4.89 | 0.75 |
C57BL/6NCrl | with | 5.99 | 4.78 | i.11 |
C57BL/6NCrl | without | 7.46 | 7.17 | ane.29 |
C57BL/6NCrl | without | 7.17 | 7.09 | 0.17 |
C57BL/6NCrl | without | 7.02 | half-dozen.04 | 0.62 |
C57BL/6NCrl | without | 6.77 | 6.54 | 0.58 |
C57BL/6NCrl | without | 6.83 | 6.65 | 0.72 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | with | 10.90 | 11.12 | 0.52 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | with | 11.93 | xi.98 | 1.75 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | with | 12.42 | 12.00 | 0.10 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | with | xi.99 | 10.61 | i.44 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | without | fifteen.00 | 14.99 | 0.11 |
Crl:CD1(Icr) | without | 11.99 | 11.69 | 0.35 |
Crl:CF1 | with | 11.91 | eleven.45 | 0.87 |
Crl:CF1 | without | 12.00 | 12.00 | 0.75 |
Crl:CFW(SW) | with | 8.74 | viii.thirteen | one.32 |
Crl:CFW(SW) | with | 8.49 | 8.23 | 0.67 |
Crl:CFW(SW) | with | 8.l | seven.86 | 0.63 |
Crl:CFW(SW) | with | eight.50 | 7.53 | 0.96 |
Crl:CFW(SW) | without | 8.75 | eight.09 | 0.72 |
DBA/2NCrl | with | 4.35 | 4.54 | 0.32 |
DBA/2NCrl | without | 4.48 | 4.48 | 0.89 |
DBA/2NCrl | without | 4.38 | iv.82 | 0.72 |
FVB/NCrl | with | 10.83 | x.45 | 0.46 |
FVB/NCrl | with | 11.02 | 11.39 | 0.26 |
FVB/NCrl | without | viii.67 | 8.63 | 0.79 |
Tabular array 2.
Litter size and mortality among rat stocks housed with or without mice
Strain or stock | Multispecies? | Mean no. of pups per litter at birth | Mean no. of pups weaned per littera b | Mean no. of pups missing per litterc |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 11.25 | 14.01 | 0.20 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.22 | eleven.43 | 0.36 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 11.96 | 13.86 | 0.13 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.18 | 13.86 | 0.fourteen |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | xi.88 | thirteen.12 | 0.27 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.93 | 13.65 | 0.forty |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.02 | 14.12 | ane.19 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.51 | 13.17 | 0.46 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | xiv.25 | 13.11 | 0.20 |
Crl:CD(SD) | with | 12.46 | thirteen.91 | 0.12 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | xi.60 | 12.18 | ane.31 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 11.29 | 13.94 | 0.28 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 12.65 | 13.83 | 0.nineteen |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 12.98 | thirteen.lxxx | 0.25 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 11.69 | 13.76 | 0.27 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 11.06 | xiii.89 | 0.43 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 12.42 | 13.69 | 0.32 |
Crl:CD(SD) | without | 12.41 | 13.97 | 0.54 |
Crl:LE | with | xiv.lx | 12.69 | 0.52 |
Crl:LE | with | 14.19 | 12.38 | 0.61 |
Crl:LE | without | 13.61 | 12.04 | 0.55 |
Crl:LE | without | 15.67 | 12.33 | 0.33 |
Crl:LE | without | 12.79 | 13.15 | 0.66 |
Crl:WI | with | 14.27 | 12.37 | 1.83 |
Crl:WI | with | 13.70 | 12.46 | 1.30 |
Crl:WI | with | xiii.64 | fourteen.15 | 0.46 |
Crl:WI | without | 12.11 | xiii.72 | 0.xvi |
Crl:WI | without | 14.30 | 13.85 | 0.26 |
For each colony, the numbers of litters born, number of pups born, number of litters weaned, number of pups weaned, and number of pups missing at weaning were obtained from colony breeding records, averaged over 13-wk periods during 2005, 2006, 2007, and 2008. From these information, the mean litter size at birth, mean litter size at weaning, and number of pups missing per litter at weaning were calculated. Pups missing per litter was calculated past dividing the number of litters born 3 wk prior by the number of pups missing at weaning. This adding captures pups missing due to loss of entire litters in the preweaning menses. The mean litter size at weaning does non capture missing litters, because litters missing entirely would non be recorded as weaned.
Growth rates of mice in the presence or absenteeism of rats were obtained from xv colonies bred in the absence of rats and thirteen colonies bred in the presence of rats. Samples of at least 800 animals per colony had been weighed weekly until the historic period of fifteen wk to assemble information on normal growth of the animals. Available data were collected betwixt 1999 and 2005 and came from strains BALB/cAnNCrl (BALB/c; iii colonies housed with rats, vi without), C57BL/6NCrl (B6NCrl; 5 with, half dozen without) and stocks Crl:CF1 (CF1; 4 with, two without), and Crl:CFW(SW) (1 with, ane without). Rat growth rates in the presence or absence of mice were obtained from fourteen colonies bred in the absenteeism of mice and 14 colonies bred in the presence of mice, including Crl:CD(SD) (CD; ix colonies housed with mice, 8 without), Crl:LE (L/E; 2 with, 3 without), LEW/Crl (LEW; 1 with, 1 without), Crl:SD (one with and ane without), and Crl:WI (Wistar; 1 with, 1 without). These data had been collected betwixt 1999 and 2007 to study normal growth.
Statistical analysis.
Breeding data.
The data on litter sizes at birth and weaning and pups missing per litter at weaning were summarized and subjected to a ii-way general linear model ANOVA, with strain and presence or absence of rats or mice as fixed-effect factors. Values were considered pregnant if the P value was less than or equal to 0.05. Residuals plots were used to ensure that the assumption of normality of the residuals and homogeneity of variance were met (as was the example). All calculations were washed using the MINITAB package (version 13; MINITAB, State Higher, PA).
Body weight analysis.
Mean body weight at weekly intervals was recorded separately for males and females for each strain or stock and and then subjected to a full general linear model ANOVA at 21, 42, 63, 84, 91, and 105 d of historic period, with the factors strain and presence or absence of the other species. Residuals plots were used to examine normality of the residuals and equality of variances in each group. Mean body weights were plotted separately for each strain of mice in the presence or absenteeism of the other species because ane stock showed anomalous results. For the rats, however, body weights were averaged across strains or stocks.
Results
Mice bred in the presence of rats.
Mouse colonies maintained with or without rats in the same room showed no pregnant differences in mean litter size at birth and weaning and pups missing at weaning (Table three). As expected, mean litter size at birth and weaning differed markedly (P < 0.001) between strains and stocks. By dissimilarity, comparisons of strains and stocks showed no significant difference (P = 0.99) in number of pups missing per litter, an indication of pups that died or were cannibalized. In addition, statistically meaning interactions between strain or stock and presence or absenteeism of rats were not detected for litter size at birth, at weaning, or pups missing (P = 0.53, 0.47, and 0.95, respectively), indicating no testify for strain differences in response of mice to rats.
Table 3.
Least-squares (weighted) means ± SEMs, conviction intervals, and P values for mice housed with or without rats
Type (and number) of colonies | No. of pups per litter at birth (hateful ± SEM) | No. of pups per litter at weaning (mean ± SEM) | No. of pups missing per litter at weaning (mean ± SEM) |
With rats (17) | viii.24 ± 0.xx | seven.98 ± 0.26 | 0.614 ± 0.131 |
Without rats (16) | viii.43 ± 0.21 | 8.25 ± 0.26 | 0.615 ± 0.133 |
Deviation betwixt colonies with or without rats | 0.18 | 0.26 | 0.001 |
95% conviction interval | −0.42 to 0.78 | −0.51 to 1.forty | −0.394 to 0.395 |
P values | |||
Multispecies housing | 0.53 | 0.47 | 0.95 |
Strain | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.99 |
Multispecies housing × strain | 0.09 | 0.21 | 0.68 |
Although information technology is impossible to evidence a negative (that is, that the presence of rats has no effect at all), the 95% confidence intervals and colony sizes analyzed advise that whatsoever differences must be quite pocket-size. For instance, mean litter size at birth has a 95% probability that the true difference between colonies with or without rats lies somewhere between 0.42 pup more than per litter when rats are present to 0.78 fewer pups per litter in the absence of rats. Therefore, despite a slight suggestion that litter size is greater in the absenteeism of rats, the possibility that litter size could exist greater in the presence of rats cannot be ruled out. A similar argument holds for litter size at weaning and missing pups per litter.
Rats bred in the presence of mice.
Similar results were seen with rats bred in the presence of mice (Table 4). Therefore there was no evidence that the presence of another species contradistinct litter size at birth and weaning, or the number of pups missing per litter at weaning (P > 0.05 in each case).
Table iv.
To the lowest degree-squares (weighted) means ± SEMs, confidence intervals, and P values for rats housed with or without mice
Type (and number) of colonies | No. of pups per litter at nativity (mean ± SEM) | No. of pups per litter at weaning (mean ± SEM) | No. of pups missing per litter at weaning (mean ± SEM) |
With mice (15) | 13.54 ± 0.28 | 12.98 ± 0.23 | 0.701 ± 0.116 |
Without mice (13) | 13.08 ± 0.28 | thirteen.31 ± 0.23 | 0.389 ± 0.118 |
Departure between colonies with or without mice | −0.46 | 0.32 | −0.312 |
95% conviction interval | −1.28 to 0.36 | −0.34 to 0.99 | −0.655 to 0.031 |
P values | |||
Multispecies housing | 0.25 | 0.33 | 0.26 |
Strain | <0.001 | 0.04 | 0.07 |
Multispecies housing × strain | 0.94 | 0.64 | 0.03 |
Torso weights of mice growing in rooms with rats.
Plots of hateful body weight of male person and female mice for 4 strains in the presence or absence of rats (that is, multispecies housing) are shown in Figures one and two. Differences between those bred in the presence or absenteeism of rats were not statistically significant for BALB/cAnNCrl, C57BL/6NCrl, and Crl:CF1, but were large with Crl:CFW(SW), which had greater growth (for instance, P < 0.01 at 91 d) in the presence of rats. Nevertheless but 2 colonies of Crl:CFW(SW) were included in the assay (1 with and 1 without rats)

Mean growth curves for male mice of 2 inbred and 2 outbred stocks reared in rooms either with or without rats. See Materials and Methods department for details of the number of groups measured for each strain or stock.

Hateful growth curves for female person mice of 2 inbred and 2 outbred stocks reared in rooms either with or without rats. See Materials and Methods section for details of the number of groups measured for each strain or stock.
Body weight of rats growing in rooms containing mice.
The growth curves of rats growing in the presence or absence of mice is shown in Figure 3, pooled across strains. Statistical analysis at 21, 42, 63, 84, and 105 d showed no evidence of any differences in growth between the 2 groups although, every bit expected, at that place were highly significant strain differences at all ages (data not shown).

Mean growth curves averaged across v stocks and strains of rats, consisting of fourteen groups with and 14 groups without mice in the aforementioned rooms. Run across Materials and Methods section for details of the number of groups measured for each strain or stock.
Discussion
In the wild, the Norwegian rat and house mouse occupy a like ecological niche. Both are rodents that can exist found in association with humans and their dwellings, leading to both their categorization as vermin and their eventual domestication. Both rats and mice are dietary generalists, eating almost anything they come beyond merely too preying on invertebrates, cold-blooded vertebrates, and animals smaller than themselves. In the case of rats, predation may include killing and consuming mice. The offset detailed description of mouse killing by wild and domesticated rats nether laboratory conditions in the literature 23 has led to many studies of the mouse-killing response in rats. Nevertheless, mice elicit a number of behavioral responses in rats, including killing, sniffing, carrying, mothering, manipulating, training, and disinterest. Not all rats kill mice, and mice are not preferred over other casualty species. When offered a choice of casualty, mice elicited a killing response in 12% to 25% of laboratory rats tested, whereas frogs and turtles were killed at a rate of almost 100%, as were cockroaches. 2,23,24 These rates of killing differ between strains or stocks, xvi,28,36,39,42 but generally do non differ between males and females inside a strain or stock 23,42 and tin can be modified by a wide variety of environmental, pharmacologic, surgical, and behavioral manipulations. fifteen,eighteen,19,30,31,35,41 Housing large numbers of mice and rearing them from nativity in proximity but non direct contact with rats, equally was the case in this report, may also be such a modifier.
Rats will act every bit foster mothers to infant mice, and weanling rats reared in the same cage with weanling mice are not reported to take damaged the mice in whatever way. 11-14,38 In addition, close exposure of mice to rats both before and afterward weaning affects the mice's behavioral patterns. These mice preferred rats and displayed lower intraspecies aggression. 11 In another written report, mice reared by rats or in the presence of a rat 'aunt' had a decreased stress response to a novel stimulus, but this decreased response was dependent on straight physical contact between the two species. 11,fourteen
A relatively recent review of the furnishings of predator odors on mammalian prey species does not mention the effects of rat odors on mice, but the odors of other more strictly predatory species do have a suppressive effect on rodent reproduction. 1 Work straight examining the effect of rat exposure on pregnancy in mice found a decline in reproductive performance as measured past number of pups produced when a female mouse was exposed to a rat during the commencement 7 d later on mating. ten Exposure in the cited study consisted of 1 of iii conditions: housing a newly mated mouse in the same muzzle equally a nonkilling male rat; housing a newly mated mouse in the same muzzle as a nonkilling male rat but separating the 2 with a wire grid; and exposure of a newly mated mouse to rat urine. Both outbred and inbred (CD1 and C57, the classification used in the study cited) mice were tested; outbred mice were less affected by the presence of the rat. 10 Exposing female mice directly to odors of male rats may act through pathways other than those associated with stress, because the presence of a male mouse in the cage of a recently impregnated female person mouse removed the consequence of rats entirely. 25 The Bruce effect (failure of implantation in female person mice exposed to a strange male) is well-documented, and pheromones produced by male rats may affect female mice in this fashion. Because our report was not prospective, nosotros cannot argue against the thought that a decline in reproductive functioning might occur if a mouse colony naïve to rats is moved to a room with rats or if a new room is established with minor colonies of both mice and rats. The colonies examined in this work were well-established every bit described and had been stable for several years at the time of data collection.
The growth of animals is some other concrete parameter affected by stress. Rats and mice stressed prenatally testify deviations in growth. Depending on the level of stress, type of stress, and bespeak in gestation at which the stress is administered, dams and offspring may show either growth retardation or augmentation of growth. 20,26,34 Stress has been shown to decrease body weight in both rats and mice, 21,32 merely sure types of stress accept also been shown to increase trunk weight. 33 The lack of difference in the growth rates of both rats and mice in the current study argues that animals are experiencing similar levels of stress, regardless of whether other species are present. Thus, some factor other than the presence of rats may have accounted for the unusually large intercolony variation detected in our assay.
When taken alone, the early on work 23 documenting a theoretical predator–prey relationship between rats and mice has led researchers to posit that mice find the presence of rats stressful. When exposure to rats is used as a source of stress for mice, paradigms include singly housing naïve mice for a express fourth dimension period (less than 30 d) in open-topped cages in a room containing male person rats, 6,9 placing mice in the same cage equally rats, x exposing singly housed mice to a rat in a dissever compartment, five,44 or exposing singly housed mice to a rat fecal bolus or rat urine. 25,29 Based on the results of these studies, some authors draw the decision that exposing naïve mice to or housing mice with rats is stressful.
Two factors, withal, confound that conclusion. The assumption of mouse naïveté in relation to rat exposure may be wrong. In at to the lowest degree 1 rat exposure written report, outbred CD1 mice were obtained from a facility where mice and rats are reared together in the same room, and whether mice were obtained specifically from a room not containing rats is non mentioned. 29 Mice built-in and reared in the presence of rats may feel different stress levels when exposed to rats than those introduced after in life. The second gene relates to the social nature of mice. Rat exposure studies often use singly housed mice and rarely use group-housed or grouping-exposed experimental or control groups. One study that used group-housed unexposed controls, measured sympathetic neurotransmitter release in mice exposed to rats. 9 The response of mice housed in groups and exposed to rat odors and vocalizations was not different from the control group, whereas both mice singly housed and mice singly housed and exposed to rats differed significantly from the command. 9 Single housing for a social species such as mice can itself be a stressor 4,8 and could predispose these animals to an exaggerated response to the presence of predators 3 or even outcome in the changes seen. nine Because strain-dependent responses to stress are well-documented in the literature, 27,37 the response to rat exposure could too vary betwixt strains and stocks of mice.
The common assumptions are that a predator–casualty relationship always occurs between rats and mice due to their difference in size and their wild-ancestral behavioral tendencies and that mice cannot acclimate to the presence of rats. Fifty-fifty ignoring the fact that laboratory rodents are domesticated (not wild) animals and that they may preserve many behaviors of their wild ancestors, this idea seems to be faux. For example, rats and mice appear to acclimate to the presence of humans, a noteworthy, admitting perhaps not 'natural,' predator of both species. In fact, early exposure to humans reduced the rate of muricide in rats. 17 Perhaps other factors in a production environment permit for acclimation or adaptation. Many strains of mice are bullheaded, visually impaired, or deaf. 7,40,43 Rats and mice are non housed together in the aforementioned cage, so their primary experience of rats would be via scent. In the facilities giving rise to the data evaluated herein (Charles River), the cages are open-topped, merely animals are separated by meter-wide aisles, and differing cage sizes for each species means that rats and mice are not housed on the same rack. The scents of thousands of other conspecifics may overwhelm the scent of rats in the aforementioned room. These factors may mitigate odor exposure. Alternatively, if mice housed with rats in the same secondary enclosure tin notice rat odors, peradventure the large number of conspecifics is a protective factor, offering the perception that the gamble to any particular mouse is reduced. In product atmospheric condition, animals are rarely singly housed, which may also provide a protective effect. I study showed that closer olfactory, auditory, and visual contact than that which is present in standard Charles River mouse and rat housing did not consequence in changes in the corticosterone response of mice to a novel stimulus, 14 indicating that housing animals in the same room would not tend to potentiate changes in fright- or stress-based behavior. In many research facilities, rodents are housed in ventilated racks, theoretically further reducing olfactory and auditory exposure to other rodents. Before housing recommendations are made by governing bodies or institutions, further studies should investigate housing modalities in use and attempt to decide whether housing type affects potential interaction between mice and rats in a significant and reproducible fashion.
A last question is raised by this test of housing rats and mice together: Exercise rats notice the presence of mice stressful? Because rats announced to carry and perform similarly whether housed in a room lone or together with mice, the present study shows that, at least for growth and reproductive parameters, the data do non support any direct effects.
The lack of consistency in findings of other studies coupled with the relatively pocket-size numbers of both animals and stocks and strains examined in those previous studies does non back up a ban on housing rats and mice of the same health profile in the same secondary enclosure. The results of the present study, which used large numbers of animals, further the contention that housing rats and mice in the same secondary enclosure is not a harmful do. Housing convenance rats and mice in the same room does not have an effect on the growth or reproduction of either species, and data collected in this study practice not support the contention that such housing is stressful.
References
1. Apfelbach R, Blanchard CD, Blanchard RJ, Hayes RA, McGregor IS. 2005. The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: a review of field and laboratory studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:1123–1144 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2. Bandler R, Jr, Moyer KE. 1970. Animals spontaneously attacked by rats. Commun Behav Biol 5:177–182 [Google Scholar]
3. Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Parmigiani Southward. 2002. Group housed mice: are they really stressed? Ethol Ecol Evol 14:341–350 [Google Scholar]
4. Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Sacerdote P, Ceresini G, Chirieleison A, Panerai AE, Parmigiani S. 2003. Individual housing induces altered immunoendocrine responses to psychological stress in male mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28:540–558 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
5. Beekman M, Flachskamm C, Linthorst AC. 2005. Effects of exposure to a predator on behaviour and serotonergic neurotransmission in different encephalon regions of C57BL/6N mice. Eur J Neurosci 21:2825–2836 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
half-dozen. Calvo-Torrent A, Brain PF, Martinez M. 1999. Effect of predatory stress on sucrose intake and beliefs on the plus maze in male mice. Physiol Behav 67:189–196 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
7. Chang B, Hawes NL, Hurd RE, Davisson MT, Nusinowitz Southward, Heckenlively JR. 2002. Retinal degeneration mutants in the mouse. Vision Res 42:517–525 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
viii. Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R, Gass P. 2005. Social and structural housing conditions influence the development of a depressive-like phenotype in the learned helplessness paradigm in male mice. Behav Encephalon Res 164:100–106 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
ix. D'Arbe Thousand, Einstein R, Lavidis NA. 2002. Stressful animal housing weather and their potential upshot on sympathetic neurotransmission in mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 282:R1422–R1428 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
ten. de Catanzaro D. 1988. Effect of predator exposure upon early pregnancy in mice. Physiol Behav 43:691–696 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
11. Denenberg VH, Hudgens GA, Zarrow MX. 1964. Mice reared with rats: modification of behavior by early on feel with some other species. Scientific discipline 143:380–381 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
12. Denenberg VH, Hudgens GA, Zarrow MX. 1966. Mice reared with rats: effects of mother on adult behavior patterns. Psychol Rep 18:451–456 [Google Scholar]
thirteen. Denenberg VH, Paschke RE, Zarrow MX. 1973. Mice reared with rats: effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal environments upon hybrid offspring of C57BL/10J and Swiss Albino mice. Dev Psychobiol six:21–31 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
fourteen. Denenberg VH, Paschke R, Zarrow MX, Rosenberg KM. 1969. Mice reared with rats: emptying of odors, vision, and audition as significant stimulus sources. Dev Psychobiol ii:26–28 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
15. Depaulis A, Vergnes Grand. 1984. GABAergic modulation of mouse-killing in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 83:367–372 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
16. Eichelman B. 1980. Variability in rat irritable and predatory aggression. Behav Neural Biol 29:498–505 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
17. Garbanati JA, Sherman GF, Rosen GD, Hofmann K, Yutzey DA, Denenberg VH. 1983. Handling in infancy, brain laterality, and muricide in rats. Behav Brain Res 7:351–359 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
18. Giammanco Southward, Ernandes Thou, Paderni MA. 1990. Environmental lighting and muricidal behaviour in the male Wistar rat. Arch Int Physiol Biochim 98:19–21 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
19. Gomita Y, Ueki S. 1980. Furnishings of limbic lesions, peculiarly of olfactory bulbectomy, on simple conditioned avoidance response in rats. J Pharmacobiodyn 3:94–104 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
20. Gotz AA, Wolf G, Stefanski V. 2008. Psychosocial maternal stress during pregnancy: effects on reproduction for F0 and F1 generation laboratory rats. Physiol Behav 93:1055–1060 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. Harris RB, Zhou J, Youngblood BD, Rybkin II, Smagin GN, Ryan DH. 1998. Consequence of repeated stress on body weight and body limerick of rats fed low- and high-fat diets. Am J Physiol 275:R1928–R1938 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
22. Institute of Laboratory Animal Resource 1996. Guide for the care and utilise of laboratory animals. Washington (DC): National University Press [Google Scholar]
23. Karli P. 1956. The Norway rat'southward killing response to the white mouse: an experimental analysis. Behaviour 10:81–102 [Google Scholar]
24. Kemble ED, Flannelly KJ, Salley H, Blanchard RJ. 1985. Mouse killing, insect predation, and conspecific assail by rats with differing prior aggressive experience. Physiol Behav 34:645–648 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
25. Kumar A, Dominic CJ. 1996. Rat-induced disruption of early pregnancy in mice and its prevention by the stud male. Indian J Exp Biol 34:191–196 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Lee YE, Byun SK, Shin S, Jang JY, Choi BI, Park D, Jeon JH, Nahm SS, Kang JK, Hwang SY, Kim JC, Kim YB. 2008. Effect of maternal restraint stress on fetal development of ICR mice. Exp Anim 57:xix–25 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Liu 10, Gershenfeld HK. 2003. An exploratory cistron analysis of the tail suspension test in 12 inbred strains of mice and an F2 intercross. Brain Res Bull 60:223–231 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
28. McMillen BA, Chamberlain JK, DaVanzo JP. 1988. Effects of housing and muricidal beliefs on serotonergic receptors and interactions with novel anxiolytic drugs. J Neural Transm 71:123–132 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
29. Merali Z, Levac C, Anisman H. 2003. Validation of a simple, ethologically relevant image for assessing anxiety in mice. Biol Psychiatr 54:552–565 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
30. Miachon Southward, Cespuglio R. 1997. Prevention of ACTH- and adrenalectomy-induced muricidal behavior: past benzodiazepinic ligands. Peptides 18:185–189 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
31. Miachon Due south, Claustrat B, Cespuglio R. 1995. Consecration of muricidal beliefs past ACTH or adrenalectomy in young male Wistar rats. Brain Res Balderdash 36:119–123 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
32. Michel C, Duclos M, Cabanac Chiliad, Richard D. 2005. Chronic stress reduces trunk fat content in both obesity-prone and obesity-resistant strains of mice. Horm Behav 48:172–179 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
33. Moles A, Bartolomucci A, Garbugino L, Conti R, Caprioli A, Coccurello R, Rizzi R, Ciani B, D'Amato FR. 2006. Psychosocial stress affects energy residuum in mice: modulation by social status. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31:623–633 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
34. Mueller BR, Bale TL. 2006. Bear on of prenatal stress on long term torso weight is dependent on timing and maternal sensitivity. Physiol Behav 88:605–614 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
35. Paul L, Miley WM, Mazzagatti Northward. 1973. Social facilitation and inhibition of hunger-induced killing by rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 84:162–168 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
36. Potegal Yard, Myers MM. 1989. Spontaneously hypertensive Wistar-derived male rats are more ambitious than those of their normotensive progenitor strain. Behav Neural Biol 51:247–261 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Pothion S, Bizot JC, Trovero F, Belzung C. 2004. Strain differences in sucrose preference and in the consequences of unpredictable chronic mild stress. Behav Brain Res 155:135–146 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
38. Rosenberg KM, Denenberg VH, Zarrow MX. 1970. Mice (Mus musculus) reared with rat aunts: the part of rat–mouse contact in mediating behavioural and physiological changes in the mouse. Anim Behav 18:138–143 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
39. Tingstrom DH, tertiary, Thorne BM. 1978. The effects of colony differences on muricidal beliefs in rats inside one strain. J Gen Psychol 98:23–29 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
40. Turner JG, Parrish JL, Hughes LF, Toth LA, Caspary DM. 2005. Hearing in laboratory animals: strain differences and nonauditory effects of noise. Comp Med 55:12–23 [PMC gratis article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
41. Vergnes Thou, Depaulis A, Boehrer A, Kempf Eastward. 1988. Selective increase of offensive behavior in the rat following intrahypothalamic v,vii-DHT-induced serotonin depletion. Behav Brain Res 29:85–91 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
42. Walsh LL. 1982. Strain and sexual practice differences in mouse killing past rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 96:278–283 [Google Scholar]
43. Wong AA, Brown RE. 2006. Visual detection, pattern discrimination, and visual acuity in 14 strains of mice. Genes Brain Behav five:389–403 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
44. Yang M, Augustsson H, Markham CM, Hubbard DT, Webster D, Wall PM, Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. 2004. The rat exposure examination: a model of mouse defensive behaviors. Physiol Behav 81:465–473 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
lovelaceprideaped.blogspot.com
Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2755018/
Belum ada Komentar untuk "Breading Mice Should You Take Out Old Babys When New Ones Arive"
Posting Komentar